Surveying medical interns’ visual short-term memory and response
inhibition function before and after a night shift, sleep quality and smoking
habits in Rasht 2020: a cross-sectional study
Zoheir Reihanian 1, Ali Dolat 2, Ali Ashraf
2*
1 Neuroscience
Research Center, School of Medicine, Guilan University of Medical Sciences,
Rasht, Iran
2 Clinical Research Development Unit of Poursina Hospital, Guilan
University of Medical Sciences, Rasht, Iran
*Corresponding
Author: Ali Ashraf
* Email: crdu_poursina@gums.ac.ir
Abstract
Introduction: Physicians whether during training years or working years are involved
with the shift work system. Night shifts and the sleep deprivation that ensues
have many adverse effects both physical and mental. Hence any decrease in
doctors’ mental capacities will potentially put his and his patients’ health at
risk, we decided to conduct a study to check medical interns’ Visual Short-Term
Memory (VSTM) and Response Inhibition Functions changes after a night shift
compared to the day before.
Materials
and Methods: In this study, 32 medical interns from the Guilan University of Medical
Sciences were recruited by random sampling. Each participant completed a
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index questionnaire and was asked about his/her
smoking habit and prior night's sleep. Each examinee then was tested in the
morning before and after the night shift for VSTM by forward Corsi Block
Tapping Test (CBTT) and for Response Inhibition by Stroop Word/Color Test
implemented in the PEBL software version 2beta6. The data were coded and
analyzed in SPSS v21.
Results: There was no significant change in the intern's CBTT and Stroop Test
results after a night shift and their VSTM and Response Inhibition Functions
remained fairly intact. According to their PSQI results, 29 interns had poor
sleep quality and only two had an acceptable PSQI score (5 or less).
Conclusion: We argued that the consistent results of the CBTT and the Stroop Color
Test could be due to many causes from small sample size to overall lower
function under chronic sleep deprivation.
Keywords: Night Shift, Intern, Visual Short-Term Memory, Response Inhibition,
Smoking, Sleep Quality
Introduction
Since the Libby Zion Case and the issuing of “The Law of
Libby Zion” based on Bell Commission's work, sleep deprivation (SD) due to
night shifts and its toll on doctors’ and patients’ safety and their overall
life satisfaction is of major regard for both doctors and health authorities.
High serious and fatal errors are prevalent among sleep-deprived Interns and
Residents after the regular night shifts which can easily lead to 36 or 48
hours of SD (1). A higher rate of self-injuries during invasive procedures
combined with loose sensory-motor coordination comparable to considerable blood
alcohol levels that had resulted in motor vehicle accidents during the commute
to the hospital and home makes everyone in contact with a sleepy doctor
reasonably anguished (2,3). Many scholars have studied different aspects of SD
(acute and chronic) and its effect on different functions of the human brain in
acute forms. Working memory and it's proposed two subcategories Visual Short
Term Memory and Verbal Short Term Memory are regarded as a unit that allows
representations to be actively extended over time in the absence of sensory
input, making such information available to more complex cognitive operations
ranging from mental arithmetic to problem-solving, therefore we assume it as
the main storage unit for the data gathered by history taking and physical
examination before writing them down or putting an order since short term
memory (STM) “must support memory for previously unencountered information, the
storage of multiple tokens of the same type, and variable binding” (4-6).
As the brain uses the same memory unit to analyze the data and to reference
Long Term Memory content, any shortcoming in this capacity is potentially
harmful to the efficacy and integrity of the service provided by a doctor or
the material learned by an intern or a resident. The other interesting function
that seems to keep a human on the perceived (or learned and exercised) right
track by mitigating external influence on the decisions he makes is the
response inhibition function, which we use to rethink our response to external
stimuli right before acting it out a.k.a self-control. Executive function, the
cognitive control of behavior, depends on the prefrontal cortex, which is
highly developed in higher primates and especially humans. The diverse inputs
and back projections to both cortical and subcortical structures put the
prefrontal cortex in a position to exert what is often called “top-down”
control or cognitive control of behavior (7). This executive
function is crucial to make the right decisions under pressure like when
somebody is very sleepy or a doctor is handling a rude or inappropriate
patient. Overall sleep quality of a medical student is important for him/her to
retain learned material and conduct a healthy lifestyle for years to come (8,9).
A chronic poor sleep pattern harms the hippocampus and memory system (10).
Speaking of lifestyle; Smoking cigarettes is one of the most harmful risk
factors for many health aspects, it is a treatable and preventable cause, but
beforehand we need to have a clear assessment of the situation, physicians must
be a living example of what they propagate if they believe in it. Due to the
lack of study in this area among medical students in Guilan, we decided to
conduct a study to evaluate the function of short-term visual memory and
inhibit response before and after shift and sleep quality in the internships of
educational
hospitals in Rasht, Iran.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Conducted a cross-sectional analytical study among 32
interns in Poursina Hospital in Rasht during June and July of 2020. Sampling
was done by asking available interns if they are interested in participating.
The use of self-report questionnaires was limited by clarifying the questions
and selecting options that have limited interpretation. Lack of control over
variables such as sleep patterns and recreation of the subjects included in the
study, which tried to reduce their effect by defining appropriate exclusion
criteria. Fatigue and lack of cooperation of the interns after the watch and
tightness of the intern program in the hospital were other problems of the
project. The exclusion criteria were being on any long-term medication, already
having sleeping problems that require using sleeping aids agents and not
adhesion to the complete study. We used a checklist of two questions regarding
being a smoker and starting period of smoking regularly.
Corsi Block Tapping Test (CBTT)
We conducted a
session of testing visuospatial short-term working memory using PEBL (v2.6
portable) software running on an iLife ZedAir2(™) notebook with 14” screen and
an ordinary wireless mouse.
Stroop Color and Word Test
(SCWT): We used Corsi Block Tapping Test (forward) and Stroop Word Color Test to
gather data about the examinee’s VSTM and Response Inhibition Function.
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)
We use 19-item self-rated questionnaire pittsburg sleep
quality index. It assesses subjective sleep quality in the previous month. The
total score ranges from 0 to 21, with higher scores indicating poorer sleep
quality. The reliability and validity of the Persian version of the PSQI have
been assessed previously in Iran. A total PSQI score > 5 indicates poor
sleep quality Thus, those with a PSQI of less than five were considered good
sleepers.
All the tests were conducted in a quiet room with an air
conditioner and a suitable desk and chair to accommodate the examinee's height
and eliminate intruding factors as much as possible. Interns were willing to be
tested after the morning report and having their breakfast and we had to
comply.
Sample size
In determining the sample size based on Sarabadani et
al.'s study with 95% confidence interval and 90% power, due to the inverse
relationship between sleep quality and working memory in students (-0.52), we
reached the number 32 as sample size.
Statistical Analysis
The statistical analysis was performed using SPSS v21
software. Mean and standard deviation (95% confidence interval) were used to
describe quantitative variables with normal distribution and median and mean
range were used for quantitative variables with the abnormal distribution.
Qualitative variables were also described based on number and percentage. The
normal distribution of the study quantitative variables was measured using
elongation and skewness values, histogram diagram, Q-Q plot diagram and
Shapiro-Wilk test. To compare abnormal quantitative variables of visuospatial
short-term working memory score and response inhibition score before and after
internship shifts using nonparametric Wilcoxon equivalent.
Results
From
the total of 32 interns were entered the study, the results of one intern were
excluded due to the exclusion criterion of “regular use of sleep aid agents”.
The mean age of 31 participants was 25.42±0.720 years with ages 24 to 27 years.
Descriptive results which are shown in table 1, exhibited 20 (64.5%) interns
were male and 11 (34.5%) interns were female. 14 (45.2%) were smokers with the
majority beginning smoking from Physio path/Stager 6 (19.4%). More than half of
the participants 16 (51.6%) had slept 4-6 hours during the last night and the
duration of the night shift sleeping of 21 (67.7%) of them was 0-2 hours.
Table 1. Descriptive result of medical
interns.
Age (year) |
Mean ± SD |
|
|
25.42±0.720 |
|
Sex N (%) |
||
|
Female |
11 (35.5) |
|
Male |
20
(64.5) |
Smoking, n (%) |
||
|
Yes |
14
(45.2) |
|
No |
17 (54.8) |
Beginning of smoking, n (%) |
||
|
high school |
3 (9.7) |
|
Basic Science |
3
(9.7) |
|
Physio path/Stager |
6 (19.4) |
|
Internship |
2
(6.5) |
Sleep duration last night
(hour) |
||
|
2-4 |
3
(9.7) |
|
4-6 |
16 (51.6) |
|
> 6 |
12
(38.7) |
Sleep duration night shift (hour) |
||
|
no sleep |
5
(16.1) |
|
0-2 |
21 (67.7) |
|
2-4 |
4
(12.9) |
|
4-6 |
1 (3.2) |
Results of pre-shift and post-shift examinations
regarding the visuospatial short-term working memory score and response
inhibition score shows in table 2. The Wilcoxon signed rank test on the Corsi
block-tapping test and Stroop color-word test exhibited no significant change
before and after the night shift. The mean PSQI was 9.0±2.49 with poor sleep
quality (PSQI > 5) only in 2 (6.5%) interns.
Table 2. The difference of visuospatial
short-term working memory score according to CBTT and response inhibition score
according to SCWT before and after the night shift in medical interns.
|
|
Before shift |
After shift |
Negative Rank |
Positive Rank |
Ties |
Za |
P |
|
|
|
Median (IQR) |
Median (IQR) |
||||||
Corsi block-tapping test |
|
||||||||
|
Block span |
8 (6-8) |
11 (9-11) |
5 |
5 |
21 |
-0.105 |
0.917 |
|
|
Total score |
88 (54-88) |
6.5 (5.5-6.5) |
6 |
14 |
11 |
-1.046 |
0.295 |
|
|
Correct trial |
8 (6-8) |
11 (10-12) |
6 |
14 |
11 |
-1.454 |
0.145 |
|
|
Memory span |
88 (60-96) |
6.5 (6-7) |
6 |
14 |
11 |
-1.599 |
0.110 |
|
Stroop
color-word test |
|
||||||||
|
MRTC |
726 (674-812) |
747 (654-779) |
15 |
15 |
1 |
-0.113 |
0.910 |
|
|
MRTI |
766 (709-824) |
794 (725-886) |
17 |
14 |
0 |
-0.549 |
0.583 |
|
|
MRTN |
760 (689-812) |
752 (677-837) |
16 |
15 |
0 |
-0.245 |
0.806 |
|
CBTT: Corsi Block Tapping
Test; SCWT: Stroop Color and Word Test; IQR: Inter Quartile Range; a Wilcoxon
Signed Rank test. M: mean R: response T: time C: congruent I: incongruent N:
neutral. |
|
||||||||
Discussion
The consistency of CBTT and Stroop Word/Color Test among
examined interns was contrary to our initial expectations. These results could
be due to the small sample size. Conducting the test before and after a shift
with the same equipment and in the same room suggests that there is little
chance for a systematic error; as we assigned random available Interns to the
study and sampling was by choosing available random samples.
The second possible explanation is that due to changes in
health facilities priorities and more workload and responsibilities put on
residents in the COVID-19 era, interns were not as engaged in the shift work
and direct contact with the patients as before due to safety regulations and
therefore didn’t get exhausted.
The other interesting interpretation that we put forward
is, by combining the results of these two tests with PSQI results we suggest
that the overall and long-term poor sleep quality of the medical interns had
put them in a steady low functional capacity that there was no room for
reduction of VSTM span or Increase in Response Inhibition Errors and Response
Times after one night of acute SD, meaning they could not manifest their best
performance cause their night sleep was part of a vicious continuous cycle of
poor sleep quality and wouldn’t give them enough rest to retrieve their full
functional capacity.
45.2% smoker prevalence is quite high for a sample of
medical interns and it is highly skewed toward smokers. But regarding the
pattern of increase in smokers ratio from 16.6%in 2004 to 23% in 20101
and no existing program to actively target this problem, probably the high
ratio reported by this study is not far away from reality (11,12).
Combining the results of PSQI scores and the
self-declared sleep hours during the night before shift night opens a window
into the wrong habit of self-imposed chronic sleep deprivation and derangement
in “Sleep Duration” and “Sleep Efficiency” components of sleep hygiene and the
necessity of intervention. Limitation of sample size and time limit The
availability of interns for review is one of the most important limitations in
this study. Also, the impossibility of repeating the test several times and the
safe presence in the hospital environment were other limitations of this study.
Focusing the study on surgical interns with the aim of
homogenizing the results and better examining them deprived this study of
having insight and judgment about the condition of interns in other areas (such
as internal, cardiac, ENT, etc.). Insomnia is a part such as the interior having
a significant impact on the performance of interns.
Also, reducing the burden of internships and the
permanent presence of residents (especially first-year residents) has been
effective in reducing internship fatigue and not changing test results.
The publication of the results of the present study can
provide a basis for comparing future studies in each of the areas of smoking or
sleep quality with the comparison of the time of this study with the future or
statistical samples different from the sample of this study.
Also, educational planners use these results or review
the form and content of this study to make more informed decisions about
training planning, changing the number of interns on duty and training items
provided the day after the intern interns.
Conclusions
Consistent
results of CBTT and Stroop Color Test show that Acute Sleep Deprivation during
a night shift among surveyed interns did not affect their VSTM and response
inhibition and we suggest it may be the effect of chronic sleep deprivation or
the changes due to COVID-19 pandemic caused it. Poor sleep quality and the high
prevalence of cigarette smoking among interns need appropriate interference and
more research.
Author contribution
ZR and AD
wrote and compiled this article. AA wrote and edited the manuscript
comprehensively. All authors confirmed the final version of the paper.
Acknowledgments
Special
thanks to the Clinical Research Development Unit of Poursina Hospital,
especially to Mrs. Sedigheh Samimian for her generous guidance and timeless
efforts and proceeding.
Ethical Considerations
The
proposal of this study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Guilan
University of Medical Sciences IR.GUMS.REC.1399.155.
Conflict of interest
The
authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.
References
1. Barger LK, Cade BE, Ayas NT, Cronin JW, Rosner
B, Speizer FE, Czeisler CA; Harvard Work Hours, Health, and Safety Group.
Extended work shifts and the risk of motor vehicle crashes among interns. N
Engl J Med. 2005 Jan 13;352(2):125-34.
2. Melissa M. Halbach, MD, Cyril O. Spann, MD, and Glen Egan, PhD.
Effect of sleep deprivation on medical resident and student cognitive function:
A prospective study. Am J Obstet Gynecol. Volume 188, Number 5: 1198-1201
3.
Basner M, Dinges DF, Shea JA, Small DS, Zhu J, Norton L, Ecker AJ, Novak C,
Bellini LM, Volpp KG. Sleep and alertness in medical interns and residents: an
observational study on the role of extended shifts. Sleep. 2017Apr 1; 40(4).
4. Fürst AJ, Hitch
GJ. Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory
in mental arithmetic. Mem Cognit 2000; 28:774-82.
5. Gilhooly K, Logie R, Wetherick N,
Wynn V. Working memory and strategies in syllogistic-reasoning tasks. Mem
Cognit 1993; 21:115-24.
6. Norris, D.
(2017). Short-term memory and long-term memory are still different.
Psychological Bulletin, 143(9), 992-1009.
7. Malenka RC,
Nestler EJ, Hyman SE (2009). "Chapter 13: Higher Cognitive Function and
Behavioral Control". In Sydor A, Brown RY (eds.). Molecular
Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York:
McGraw-Hill Medical. pp. 313–321
8.
Berry
JA, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Chakraborty M, Davis RL. Sleep facilitates memory by
blocking dopamine neuron-mediated forgetting. Cell. 2015 Jun 18;161(7):1656-67.
9.
St-Onge MP, Grandner MA, Brown D, Conroy MB, Jean-Louis G, Coons M, Bhatt DL.
Sleep duration and quality: impact on lifestyle behaviors and cardiometabolic
health: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association.
Circulation. 2016 Nov 1;134(18): e367-86.
10. Toni-Moi Prince and Ted Abel. The impact of sleep loss on
hippocampal function. Learning and Memory. 2013. 20: 558-569
11. Seyed Fazel Pour SF, Moghaddam Nia MT, Nasir Zadeh F. Study on
attitude of students in Gilan University of Medical Sciences toward smoking.
Journal of Legal Medicine of Islamic Republic of Iran. 2004; 33(10):25-29. 11.
12.
Ghodsi H, Mokhtari N, Asiri SH. Prevalence and correlates of cigarette smoking
among students of Guilan University of Medical Sciences. Journal of Holistic
Nursing And Midwifery.2011.67(22). pp:38-43.